Charles Bonnet syndrome: low awareness among patients with glaucoma
Letter to the Editor

Charles Bonnet syndrome: low awareness among patients with glaucoma

Stellan Molander1, Dorothea Peters1,2, Amardeep Singh2,3

1Department of Ophthalmology, Skåne University Hospital, Lund, Malmö, Sweden; 2Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Ophthalmology, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden; 3Department of Ophthalmology, Rigshospitalet-Glostrup, Glostrup, Denmark

Correspondence to: Stellan Molander, MD. Department of Ophthalmology, Skåne University Hospital, Lund, Malmö, Sweden. Email: stellanmolander@gmail.com.

Received: 14 July 2020; Accepted: 03 August 2020; Published: 15 September 2020.

doi: 10.21037/aes-20-115


Charles Bonnet syndrome (CBS) is a benign condition characterised by complex visual hallucinations in individuals with visual impairment (1). Approximately one-third of affected patients suffer from distress as a direct consequence of their hallucinatory experiences (2). The distress is often caused by fear of impending insanity, and may be propagated by lack of awareness and knowledge about CBS (3). The proportion of patients with glaucoma that are familiar with CBS is unknown. Therefore, we sought to investigate this in a population with primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG).

Our study was part of a larger, yet unpublished, prospective study of the prevalence and clinical characteristics of CBS in POAG. Patients with a known diagnosis of POAG that had visited the glaucoma clinic at Skåne University Hospital in Malmö, Sweden, between April 1st 2018 and December 31st 2018 were included. We excluded patients with concurrent neurological and/or psychiatric disorders, or other known causes of vision loss, e.g. late-stage age-related macular degeneration. We explored the patients’ precursory knowledge of CBS using the question (translated to Swedish): “Some patients with poor eyesight see things which they know are not there. These hallucinations occur on repeated occasions and are described as being vivid and complex, e.g., persons, animals, flowers, or patterns. This phenomenon is known as Charles Bonnet syndrome. Have you ever heard or read about this?” in a written questionnaire (4). Patients responding affirmatively were contacted and further interviewed. Results from the patients’ visual acuity (VA) examinations and automated visual field tests were obtained from the medical records. The results closest in time to when the patient had responded to the questionnaire were chosen. Best corrected visual acuity (BCVA) was tested in both eyes using an electronic Snellen chart, and the automated visual field test 24-2 or 30-2 was conducted using the Swedish interactive thresholding algorithm on the Humphrey Field Analyser (Carl Zeiss Meditec, Dublin, CA, USA). Patients with suspected CBS also underwent objective examination of both maculae using Swept Source - Optical Coherence Tomography (Triton, Topcon, Medical Systems, Tokyo, Japan).

Statistical analysis was performed using the SPSS software (version 25, IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). For categorical data Fisher's exact test or Pearson’s chi-square test were used when the data sample was small or large, respectively. For continuous non-parametric variables, the Mann-Whitney U test was applied. A significance level of <0.05 was employed.

A total of 338 patients with definitive glaucoma, i.e., confirmed glaucomatous visual field defects on at least two consecutive tests, were included in this study. Twelve (3.5%) patients claimed they had precursory knowledge of CBS when answering the written questionnaire. Four (1,2%) of these did, in fact, exhibit prior knowledge of CBS when contacted and interviewed. Two declared knowledge of CBS because a close relative with AMD had experienced visual hallucinations. One acquired knowledge through the internet and one was informed when she consulted a nurse in the eye department by phone due to visual hallucinations. One patient did not answer the phone despite repeated attempts. The remaining seven patients stated they had misinterpreted the questionnaire or could not remember the reason for their answer.

There were no statistical significant differences in age, gender, visual field parameters, best corrected visual acuity, laterality of glaucoma, or macular pathology between those who had knowledge of CBS and those that did not (Table 1).

Table 1

Characteristics and visual function of study participants

Familiarity with CBS P-value
No (n=334) Yes (n=4)
Age, years (median, IQR) 78 (72–84) 83 (75–86) 0.33°
Male gender (n (%) 150 (44.9%) 2 (50%) 1.00*
Mean Deviation (MD), decibel (dB)
   Best seeing eye (median IQR) –4.69 (–10.9 to–1.62) –3.49 (–22.69 to –0.85) 0.87°
   Worse seeing eye (median, IQR) –15.29# (–24.70 to –8.11) –14.34# (–31.57 to –11.64) 0.51°
Best corrected visual acuity
   Best seeing eye (median, IQR) 0.9 (0.7–1.0) 0.6 (0.6–0.9) 0.19°
   Worse seeing eye (median, IQR) 0.6 (0.4–0.8) 0.5 (0.5–0.5) 0.51°
Visual impairment
   VA <0.3 in at least one eye (n, %) 84 (25.1) 2 (50.0) 0.27*
Diagnosis of glaucoma
   Bilateral (n, %) 210 (62.9) 3 (75.0) 0.151*
   Unilateral (n, %) 124 (37.1) 1 (25.0)
Other macular disease (in at least one eye) [D1]
   Early AMD (n, %) 65 (19.5) 1 (25.0) 0.960§
   Epiretinal fibrosis (n, %) 17 (5.1) 0 (0.0)
   Other (n, %) 3 (0.9) 0 (0.0)

*, Fisher’s Exact test; °, Mann-Whitney test, §Pearson Chi square test; #, Missing MD values in 30 participants without familiarity with CBS and 1 participants with familiarity with CBS due to end-stage glaucoma; , Best corrected visual acuity equal to or less than finger counting in 27 participants without familiarity with CBS and 2 participants with familiarity with CBS; [D1], according to the clinical records, patients with wet AMD or geographical AMD excluded.

According to our study, only 1.2% of patients with a diagnosis of POAG are familiar with CBS. This is a perturbingly low figure even compared to patients with late-stage AMD (where only 12% are aware of CBS) (5). The low degree of knowledge of CBS among visually impaired patients is presumably due to a lack of information and patient education. One reason for this may be that awareness of CBS is inadequate also among physicians and healthcare workers. Another reason could be that physicians and healthcare workers allot less significance to CBS as it is generally considered to be a benign condition. The degree of knowledge of CBS among ophthalmologists has not been investigated, but among general physicians it has been found to be low (6).

The prevalence of glaucoma is approximately 6% in patients aged 70 or older, and the risk of visual impairment in this group is considerable (7). A significant number of glaucoma patients should therefore be at risk of experiencing CBS. Thus, it is important to be aware of the syndrome in order to give correct and comprehensible information to these patients. Considering a majority of patients are reluctant to spontaneously share their experiences, enquiry about hallucinatory symptoms is also of great importance, especially as reassurance and education about CBS may be all that is required to help reduce anxiety in most patients (8).

There were some limitations to this study. First, the results may not be applicable to all populations since there are regional differences in the amount of knowledge available online (English being the most common language). Secondly, there may be regional and/or cultural differences in the prevalence of CBS, which in turn may affect the general awareness about the condition. Moreover, selection bias may have occurred during the inclusion process as some of the patients refused to participate in the study and one patient who declared precursory knowledge of CBS was unavailable for a subsequent telephone interview. Finally, as the proportion of patients with knowledge of CBS was very low, any comparison of demographic and clinical data between patients who were familiar and patients who were not familiar with CBS, would be associated with uncertainty.

In conclusion, we report that awareness of CBS is almost absent in a population with POAG. Measures to increase general awareness should be explored further as these will help minimise distress and unnecessary investigations and referrals.


Acknowledgments

Funding: Järnhardt foundation, Foundation for Visually Impaired in Former Malmöhus County and the Foundation of Margit and Kjell Stoltz.


Footnote

Provenance and Peer Review: This article was commissioned by the editorial office, Annals of Eye Science. The article did not undergo external peer review.

Conflicts of Interest: All authors have completed the ICMJE uniform disclosure form (available at http://dx.doi.org/10.21037/aes-20-115). AS serves as an unpaid editorial board member of Annals of Eye Science from Jun 2020 to May 2022. DP reports grants from Järnhardt foundation, grants from Foundation for Visually Impaired in Former Malmöhus County, grants from Foundation of Margit and Kjell Stoltz, during the conduct of the study. The other author has no conflicts of interest to declare.

Ethical Statement: The authors are accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Open Access Statement: This is an Open Access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0), which permits the non-commercial replication and distribution of the article with the strict proviso that no changes or edits are made and the original work is properly cited (including links to both the formal publication through the relevant DOI and the license). See: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.


References

  1. Niazi S, Krogh Nielsen M, Singh A, et al. Prevalence of Charles Bonnet syndrome in patients with age-related macular degeneration: systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Ophthalmol 2020;98:121-31. [Crossref] [PubMed]
  2. Singh A, Sørensen TL. The prevalence and clinical characteristics of Charles Bonnet Syndrome in Danish patients with neovascular age-related macular degeneration. Acta Ophthalmol 2012;90:476-80. [Crossref] [PubMed]
  3. Menon GJ, Rahman I, Menon SJ, et al. Complex visual hallucinations in the visually impaired: the Charles Bonnet Syndrome. Surv Ophthalmol. 2003;48:58-72. [Crossref] [PubMed]
  4. Holroyd S, Rabins PV, Finkelstein D, et al. Visual hallucinations in patients with macular degeneration. Am J Psychiatry 1992;149:1701-6. [Crossref] [PubMed]
  5. Singh A, Subhi Y, Sørensen TL. Low awareness of the Charles Bonnet syndrome in patients attending a retinal clinic. Low awareness of the Charles Bonnet syndrome in patients attending a retinal clinic. Dan Med J 2014;61:A4770. [PubMed]
  6. Gordon KD, Felfeli T. Family physician awareness of Charles Bonnet syndrome. Fam Pract 2018;35:595-8. [Crossref] [PubMed]
  7. Peters D, Bengtsson B, Heijl A. Lifetime risk of blindness in open-angle glaucoma. Am J Ophthalmol 2013;156:724-30. [Crossref] [PubMed]
  8. Vukicevic M, Fitzmaurice K. Butterflies and black lacy patterns: the prevalence and characteristics of Charles Bonnet hallucinations in an Australian population. Clin Experiment Ophthalmol 2008;36:659-65. [PubMed]
doi: 10.21037/aes-20-115
Cite this article as: Molander S, Peters D, Singh A. Charles Bonnet syndrome: low awareness among patients with glaucoma. Ann Eye Sci 2020;5:30.

Download Citation